Posted on Leave a comment

Psychological profiles associated with mental, cognitive and brain health in middle-aged and older adults – Nature.com

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.
Advertisement
Nature Mental Health (2025)
Metrics details
Psychological characteristics are associated with varying dementia risk and protective factors. To determine whether these characteristics aggregate into psychological profiles and whether these profiles differentially relate to aging health, we conducted a cross-sectional investigation in two independent middle-aged (51.4 ± 7.0 years (mean ± s.d.); N = 750) and older adult (71.1 ± 5.9 years; N = 282) cohorts, supplemented by longitudinal analyses in the former. Using a person-centered approach, three profiles emerged in both cohorts: those with low protective characteristics (profile 1), high risk characteristics (profile 2) and well-balanced characteristics (profile 3). Profile 1 showed the worst objective cognition in older age and middle age (at follow-up), and most rapid cortical thinning. Profile 2 exhibited the worst mental health symptomology and lowest sleep quality in both older age and middle age. We identified profile-dependent divergent patterns of associations that may suggest two distinct paths for mental, cognitive and brain health, emphasizing the need for comprehensive psychological assessments in dementia prevention research to identify groups for more personalized behavior-change strategies.
The rapid growth of the aged population poses a substantial global social and economic challenge. Mental, cognitive and brain health are affected in aging, with over 20% of adults aged 60+ years living with a psychiatric or neurological disorder, including dementia1. Indeed, the prevalence of dementia is predicted to more than double over the next 20 years2. In this context, promoting healthy life years, which includes maintaining mental health and preventing age-related cognitive decline, is critical. Research urgently needs to identify modifiable factors that impact healthy aging and dementia risk, and the mechanisms through which they act, to advance early interventions for prevention. Targeting mid-to-older adults, before the extensive accumulation of neuropathology and impairment, is vital3.
The Lancet Commission on dementia prevention identified 14 potentially modifiable factors that affect dementia risk3. While eliminating these risk factors (for example, smoking, physical inactivity, social isolation) could theoretically prevent or delay up to 45% of dementias, deepening the search for factors underpinning some of these largely physical and behavioral risks will generate greater opportunity for dementia risk reduction and healthier aging.
Recent research has begun to identify psychological characteristics that are associated with increased risk of, or protection from, cognitive decline, neurodegeneration and clinical dementia4. Repetitive negative thinking5,6,7, proneness to experience distress8 (also known as neuroticism) and perceived stress9 are associated with increased risk, whereas protective characteristics include having a sense of life-purpose10 or coherence11, self-reflection12 and dispositional mindfulness13.
Up to now, psychological risk and protective factors have almost exclusively been examined independently. This approach is limiting, because psychological characteristics do not exist in isolation. It is important to understand whether psychological characteristics aggregate into different profiles and how these aggregations relate to markers of aging and dementia risk, as this knowledge could aid in the development of targeted interventions and preventive strategies. For example, are high levels of psychological risk factors more strongly associated with age-related brain and cognitive measures than lower levels of protective psychological features?
This research requires the use of person-centered approaches to identify the latent psychological profiles of individuals14. This approach contrasts with the more widely used variable-centered approach, which examines the relationships between two or more variables in a given sample. Although useful to help understand how these variables relate to each other, variable-centered approaches are unable to holistically account for interacting characteristics that may influence outcomes15. Person-centered approaches create groups of individuals (persons) based on the similarity of their responses to the variables, so that people with similar characteristics are grouped together into a profile. These profiles can then be used to examine their relationships with different outcome measures.
Our study uses a person-centered approach using latent profile analysis (LPA) to uncover hidden patterns of psychological characteristics within individuals. By identifying distinct psychological profiles and examining their associations with multimodal markers of dementia risk, we aim to elucidate how various combinations of psychological characteristics relate to mental, cognitive and brain health. This methodology allows us to move beyond traditional variable-centered approaches, offering a comprehensive understanding of the interplay between psychological characteristics and aging-related outcomes.
The objectives of the present study were twofold: (1) to use LPA to investigate whether individual psychological characteristics associated with dementia risk or protection aggregate into similar psychological profiles in two independent cohorts of cognitively unimpaired middle-aged and older adults, and (2) to examine the associations of such putative profiles with mental health, cognition, brain integrity measured via cortical thickness, and engagement in other lifestyle-related behaviors associated with dementia risk, as well as with measures of cognitive change and brain atrophy over time.
A total of 823 cognitively unimpaired participants from the Barcelona Brain Health Initiative (BBHI; including n = 750 with baseline mental, cognitive and/or brain health data and n = 533 with both magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and cognitive data at follow-up) and 282 cognitively unimpaired participants from the Medit-Ageing study were included. Baseline demographic and clinical characteristics for each cohort are provided in Table 1. The BBHI cohort were younger (51.4 ± 7.0 versus 71.1. ± 5.9 years (mean ± s.d.); W = 10,331, P < 0.001, Wilcoxon rank-sum test) and included a larger proportion of men (48.5% versus 36.9%; χ2 = 11.60, P < 0.001, chi-squared test) than the Medit-Ageing cohort.
The model fit statistics for the LPA, conducted separately in BBHI and Medit-Ageing, are presented in Supplementary Table 1. The Vuong–Lo–Medell–Rubin likelihood ratio test (VLMR-LRT) became non-significant for the four-profile solution in both samples, indicating that a three-profile solution was the best fitting. Increasing the number of profiles resulted in small class sizes (<5% of the total sample), alongside poorer fit in many of the other metrics, supporting three profiles as the most robust solution.
As shown in Fig. 1, we found three distinct profiles in both cohorts that demonstrated comparable aggregations of psychological characteristics. Compared to the other profiles, profile 1 was characterized by lower levels of positive or protective psychological characteristics, profile 2 by higher negative or psychological risk characteristics, and profile 3 by moderately high protective and moderately low risk characteristics. We labeled the three profiles as follows: ‘low protective’ (profile 1: BBHI, n = 196, 26.1%; Medit-Ageing, n = 59, 20.9%), ‘high risk’ (profile 2: BBHI, n = 149, 19.9%; Medit-Ageing, n = 64, 22.7%) and ‘well-balanced’ (profile 3: BBHI, n = 405, 54.0%; Medit-Ageing, n = 159, 56.4%). Descriptive statistics for each profile, in both cohorts, are presented in Table 1.
Psychological factors that have been associated with dementia risk in the existing literature are in shades of red, and psychological factors that have been associated with protection against dementia are in shades of green.
The results from linear regression models examining the cross-sectional associations between psychological profile membership and mental health, cognition, lifestyle and cortical thickness after adjusting for covariates (age, sex, education and (in Medit-Ageing) study) are described below, presented in Supplementary Table 2 and displayed in Figs. 24.
Raw data distributions of depression and anxiety by profile, with the white circles representing the estimated marginal means following adjustment for covariates (age, sex and years of education, as well as study group (for Medit-Ageing data)). The 95% confidence intervals are displayed as vertical black lines. Higher scores across all measures represent greater levels of depression and anxiety. Two-tailed linear regressions were performed to test for the effect of psychological profile group membership on depression (BBHI, N = 749, F2,746 = 63.6, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, N = 282, F2,275 = 24.6, P < 0.001) and anxiety (BBHI, N = 749, F2,746 = 131.8, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, N = 281, F2,274 = 71.9, P < 0.001). A significant main effect of psychological profile is represented by a bold horizontal line at the top of the graph, with pairwise differences displayed by thinner horizontal lines below. Precise P values for pairwise comparisons are reported in Supplementary Table 2. There were no corrections for multiple comparisons. DASS-21, Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale–21 items; GDS, Geriatric Depression Scale; STAI-B, State and Trait Anxiety Inventory–Scale B; ***P < 0.001; **P < 0.01; *P < 0.05.
Raw data distributions of objective cognition and subjective cognitive complaints by profile, with the white circles representing the estimated marginal means following adjustment for covariates (age, sex and years of education, and study group (for Medit-Ageing data)). The 95% confidence intervals are displayed as vertical black lines. Higher scores across all measures represent better objective cognition and greater levels of subjective cognitive complaints (the scores for the latter are inversed for visualization purposes). Two-tailed linear regressions were performed to test for the effect of psychological profile group membership on objective cognition (BBHI, N = 729, F2,723 = 7.2, P < 0.00; Medit-Ageing, N = 280, F2,273 = 8.0, P < 0.001) and subjective cognitive complaints (BBHI, N = 738, F2,732 = 60.1, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, N = 276, F2,269 = 21.0, P < 0.001). A significant main effect of psychological profile is represented by a bold horizontal line at the top of the graph, with pairwise differences displayed by thinner horizontal lines below. Precise P values for pairwise comparisons are reported in Supplementary Table 2. There were no corrections for multiple comparisons. For visualization purposes, scores of subjective cognitive complaints for the BBHI sample were inverted from those utilized in the statistical analyses, so that higher scores reflect more subjective cognitive complaints. McNair CDS, McNair Cognitive Difficulties Scale; Neuro-QoL, Quality of Life in Neuroradiological Disorders; PACC5Abridged, Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite 5 Abridged; ***P < 0.001; **P < 0.01; *P < 0.05.
Raw data distributions of subjective sleep problems, loneliness, social network engagement and LIBRA scores by profile, with the white circles representing the estimated marginal means following adjustment for covariates (age, sex and years of education, and study group (for Medit-Ageing data)). The 95% confidence intervals are displayed as vertical black lines. Higher scores across all measures represent greater levels of subjective sleep problems and higher levels of loneliness; and higher social network and LIBRA scores indicate a larger social network and a greater dementia risk, respectively. Two-tailed linear regressions were performed to test for the effect of psychological profile group membership on subjective sleep problems (BBHI, N = 735, F2,697 = 42.1, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, N = 277, F2,270 = 15.4, P < 0.001), loneliness (BBHI, N = 703, F2,697 = 76.0, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, N = 277, F2,270 = 20.6, P < 0.001), social network engagement (BBHI, N = 738, F2,697 = 41.1, P < 0.001) and LIBRA scores (BBHI, N = 704, F2,698 = 15.7, P < 0.001). A significant main effect of psychological profile is represented by a bold horizontal line at the top of the graph, with pairwise differences displayed as thinner horizontal lines below. Precise P values for pairwise comparisons are reported in Supplementary Table 2. There were no corrections for multiple comparisons. Jenkins, Jenkins Sleep Evaluation Questionnaire; LIBRA, Lifestyle for BRAin health; LSNS, Lubben Social Network Scale; PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; UCLA, University of California Loneliness Scale; ***P < 0.001; **P < 0.01; *P < 0.05.
In both BBHI and Medit-Ageing, psychological profile membership was associated with anxiety (BBHI, F2,746 = 63.6, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, F2,274 = 71.9, P < 0.001) and depressive (BBHI, F2,746 = 131.8, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, F2,275 = 24.6, P < 0.001) symptoms. Planned pairwise comparisons revealed that individuals in profile 2 exhibited significantly elevated levels of depressive and anxiety symptoms in comparison to those in profiles 1 and 3, in both cohorts. Furthermore, participants in Profile 1 had higher anxiety and depressive symptoms compared to those in profile 3 in both cohorts.
Psychological profile membership was associated with a global cognitive composite sensitive to detecting and tracking preclinical Alzheimer’s disease-related decline (that is, the four-item ‘abridged’ Preclinical Alzheimer’s Cognitive Composite 5 (PACC5abridged)) in both BBHI (F2,723 = 8.0, P < 0.001) and Medit-Ageing (F2,273 = 12.7, P < 0.001). Although no statistically significant differences emerged in planned pairwise comparisons in BBHI, the pattern of findings closely resembled that of Medit-Ageing. In Medit-Ageing, individuals in profile 1 had significantly worse global cognitive function (that is, lower PACC5abridged scores) compared to those in profiles 2 and 3.
In addition to associations with objective cognition, psychological profile membership was related to subjective cognition in both BBHI (F2,732 = 60.1, P < 0.001) and Medit-Ageing (F2,269 = 21.0, P < 0.001). Individuals in profile 2 reported greater perceived subjective memory concerns compared to profile 3 individuals in both cohorts. Additionally, in BBHI, individuals in profile 2 reported more memory concerns than individuals in profile 1, while profile 1 individuals reported greater concerns than those in profile 3.
Following additional adjustment for anxiety and depressive symptoms in sensitivity analyses, objective and subjective cognition results remained largely unchanged (Supplementary Table 3).
In BBHI, psychological profile membership was associated with health and lifestyle factors related to dementia risk, as captured by the late-life ‘Lifestyle for Brain Health’ composite (LIBRA; F2,698 = 15.7, P < 0.001). Planned pairwise comparisons revealed that individuals in profiles 1 and 2 had poorer health and lifestyles (that is, higher LIBRA scores) compared to those in profile 3.
Exploratory analyses revealed associations between psychological profile membership and the LIBRA constituent measures of cognitive activity, hypercholesterolemia, adherence to the Mediterranean diet and smoking. No pairwise differences were observed in relation to hypercholesterolemia and smoking; however, individuals in profiles 1 and 2 reported lower levels of cognitive activity and less adherence to the Mediterranean diet than individuals in profile 3 (Supplementary Table 4).
It was not possible to compute the LIBRA in Medit-Ageing. Instead, exploratory analyses were performed to examine the association between psychological profile membership and LIBRA components. Specifically, we focused on the components that were associated with psychological profile membership in BBHI and were also available in Medit-Ageing (that is, cognitive activity, adherence to the Mediterranean diet and smoking). Partially aligning with the findings from BBHI, psychological profile membership was associated with cognitive activity and adherence to the Mediterranean diet, but not smoking. No pairwise differences were observed in relation to cognitive activity; however, individuals in profile 1 reported greater adherence to the Mediterranean diet than individuals in profiles 2 and 3.
The relationship between psychological profile membership and subjective sleep quality, loneliness and social network engagement—health and lifestyle factors associated with dementia risk but not captured by the LIBRA—were also examined. In both cohorts, analyses revealed associations between psychological profile membership and subjective sleep quality (BBHI, F2,697 = 42.1, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, F2,270 = 15.4, P < 0.001) and loneliness (BBHI, F2,697 = 76.0, P < 0.001; Medit-Ageing, F2,270 = 20.6, P < 0.001). Planned pairwise comparisons revealed that individuals in profile 2 had worse perceived sleep quality and higher levels of loneliness compared to those in profiles 1 and 3, in both cohorts. Furthermore, in BBHI, individuals in profile 1 also reported worse sleep quality and greater loneliness than those in profile 3. In BBHI, where social network engagement was also assessed, an association was observed with psychological profile membership (F2,697 = 41.1, P < 0.001), with planned pairwise comparisons indicating that participants in profiles 1 and 2 had smaller social network engagement in comparison to those in profile 3.
Following additional adjustment for anxiety and depressive symptoms in sensitivity analyses, all results remained largely unchanged (Supplementary Table 3).
In the BBHI sample, where MRI baseline data were available for 716 participants, psychological profile membership was not associated with differences in cortical thickness (as revealed by a vertex-wise general linear model conducted on FreeSurfer).
Both cognition and MRI data were obtained for 533 BBHI participants at a follow-up assessment (profile 1, n = 139, 26.1%; profile 2, n = 101, 18.9%; profile 3, n = 293, 55.0%). Over an average follow-up period of 2.3 years (range, 0.7 to 3.4 years), there were no differences in attrition rates across psychological profiles (χ2 = 1.2, P = 0.540). Compared to the total BBHI sample, individuals with longitudinal data were on average older (T381 = 2.2, P = 0.029, t-test), but did not differ in relation to the proportion of women (χ2 = 0.1, P = 0.765) or education level (T342 = 0.4, P = 0.726).
As a sensitivity check, all baseline cross-sectional analyses were re-conducted in the BBHI subsample with longitudinal data (Supplementary Tables 5 and 6). The results mirrored those for the entire sample, showing baseline associations between psychological profile membership and mental health, cognition and lifestyle factors. Planned pairwise comparisons within the BBHI subsample remained largely consistent with the full sample, revealing that profile 1 membership was associated with the lowest levels of objective cognition, poorest health and lifestyle as measured by the LIBRA, and the smallest social network. Also, individuals in profile 2 exhibited the highest levels of depression, anxiety, loneliness, subjective cognitive concerns and the worst perceived sleep quality (Supplementary Tables 5 and 6). In addition, we did not observe differences in cortical thickness.
During the 2.3-year follow-up period, no change in global cognitive function (that is, PACC5abridged scores) was observed from baseline to follow-up (β = −0.0, P = 0.787) when analyzing longitudinal data in an adjusted linear mixed-effects model. In a separate mixed-effects model, a group-by-time interaction revealed no differences in PACC5abridged score changes according to psychological profile (F2,519 = 0.7, P = 0.519). However, although psychological profile membership was not associated with changes in PACC5abridged scores, analyses revealed stability in the association between psychological profile membership and global cognitive function. Specifically, psychological profile membership was associated with PACC5abridged scores at follow-up (F2,512 = 6.5, P = 0.002) when fitting a linear regression model. Planned pairwise comparisons revealed that individuals in profile 1 demonstrated worse global cognitive function compared to those in profile 3 (β = −0.2, P = 0.011). Following additional adjustment for anxiety and depressive symptoms in sensitivity analyses, the results remained largely unchanged.
Vertex-wise general linear models were fitted using FreeSurfer to investigate longitudinal changes in cortical thickness. During the follow-up period, cortical thinning was observed, spanning the lateral and medial parts of the frontal cortex (that is, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate and orbital cortices), the inferior parietal lobule and the precuneus/posterior cingulate region, as well as the lateral, middle and anterior parts of the temporal lobe. Other regions, such as the primary visual and motor cortices, were less affected (Supplementary Fig. 1).
Psychological profile membership was associated with change in cortical thickness from baseline to an average of 2.3 years of follow-up. Specifically, planned pairwise comparisons revealed that, compared to profile 3, individuals in profile 1 exhibited the greatest cortical thinning in the inferior and middle temporal regions and the fusiform gyri bilaterally, as well as in the lateral occipital and pericalcarine area (spanning the lingual gyrus and cuneus) in the left hemisphere. These differences were maintained after adjusting for anxiety and depressive symptoms, and in this adjusted model, differences were also observed between profiles 1 and 2 in the thinning of the inferior temporal lobe region where the former group exhibited accelerated brain atrophy (Fig. 5). In sensitivity analyses that included additional adjustments for cognitive change, differences between profiles 1 and 3 were still observed in the inferior temporal and lateral occipital regions in the primary model. In the adjusted analyses, significant differences were only observed in the inferior temporal region.
a,b, Vertex-wise symmetrized percent change maps of significant clusters surviving family-wise error multiple comparison correction in 533 participants from BBHI, for the primary model (adjusted for age, sex and education; a) and the adjusted model (further adjusted for depression and anxiety symptoms; b). Blue to light blue reflects higher cortical thickness loss for profile 1 in comparison to profile 3 (in a and upper row of b) and profile 2 (lower row of b). In a, final cluster-wise P values are <0.001 for the cluster around the right lateral occipital area, 0.034 for the cluster around the right fusiform area and <0.001 around the left inferior temporal area. In b, when comparing profile 1 versus profile 3, final cluster-wise P values are <0.001 for the cluster around the right lateral occipital area and <0.001 for the cluster around the left inferior temporal area. When comparing profile 1 versus profile 2, the final cluster-wise P value is 0.004 for the cluster around the left inferior temporal area. All analyses were performed with vertex-wise one-factor/two-level general linear models, as provided by FreeSurfer.
The present study investigated the relationships between psychological profiles, mental and cognitive health, lifestyle and brain integrity. We identified three distinct profiles based on responses to questionnaires that measured nine psychological characteristics. These characteristics were selected based on previous literature reporting their relative risk of or protection from cognitive decline and dementia. The observed psychological profiles included a first group characterized by lower scores on positive or protective psychological characteristics, a second group with high negative or psychological risk characteristics, and a third group with moderately high protective and moderately low risk characteristics. These profiles were independently observed in a middle-aged cohort and an older adult cohort, suggesting that the selected psychological characteristics appear to aggregate in a robust and reproducible manner, even in populations that differ not only in age but also in gender distribution. They were also primarily based in different countries.
In both cohorts, profile 1 comprised individuals with lower scores in questionnaires capturing psychological domains that may confer protection from dementia10,16,17, reduced expression of clinical symptoms in the face of pathology18, and better cognitive status and higher brain resilience11,19. In the older adult cohort (Medit-Ageing), individuals in profile 1, compared to those in profiles 2 and 3, exhibited worse performance on the global cognitive composite score sensitive to Alzheimer’s disease-related decline. Regarding mental health, in both cohorts, participants in profile 1 showed an intermediate level of depression and anxiety symptoms, fewer than profile 2 and generally more than profile 3. In relation to modifiable health and lifestyle factors, profile 1 participants also exhibited higher LIBRA scores (indicating greater dementia risk) and lower engagement in cognitively stimulating activities compared to profile 3 in the middle-aged cohort (that is, BBHI). Notably, while some core psychological factors characterizing this group, such as purpose in life, have been previously associated with depression and anxiety20, the majority of our findings were maintained after adjusting for these symptoms.
Previous studies have shown that some personality characteristics, such as conscientiousness and openness to experience, may confer relative protection from dementia incidence17 and that they may be related to individual variability in Alzheimer’s disease biomarker expression8. Compared to positive affect, measures of eudaimonic well-being, such as purpose and meaning in life, have also been associated with reduced dementia risk10, and both clinicopathological and brain imaging studies have indicated that individuals with high purpose in life possess greater resilience to brain pathology regarding its impact on cognitive function19,21. Building on these previous observations, the present findings further indicate that individuals with lower protective factors (profile 1) exhibit accelerated atrophy compared to the well-balanced group (profile 3). These areas included posterior temporo-occipital cortical regions previously shown to reflect age-related changes but also partially included within the cortical thinning signature of Alzheimer’s disease22,23. In summary, profile 1 showed the worst cognition in Alzheimer’s disease-sensitive domains, and greater cortical thinning including non-Alzheimer’s and Alzheimer’s-sensitive regions. In addition, compared to profile 3, this group also had a higher LIBRA risk score among middle-aged individuals as well as lower engagement in specific modifiable lifestyles (that is, lower cognitive and social activity) previously associated with relative protection against dementia3.
In our study, participants in profile 2 comprised individuals with greater proneness to distress and negative thinking styles, with high loads in brooding, worry and neuroticism. Brooding and worry are core components of repetitive negative thinking, a negative style of thinking previously proposed to be central to the accumulation of cognitive debt24 and associated with accelerated cognitive decline among older adults5. Neuroticism has also been related to higher risk of dementia17,25. Our findings revealed that, in both cohorts, profile 2 individuals exhibited the greatest symptoms of depression, anxiety and loneliness, and worst sleep quality compared with the other profiles. They also had higher memory complaints compared to profile 3. In the middle-aged cohort, profile 2 also had higher overall risk for dementia (LIBRA) compared to profile 3, and lower engagement in cognitively stimulating activities. Hence, factors involving strong subjective and emotional components, all of which have previously been associated with dementia risk3,26,27,28,29,30, appear to be central features of this group. Interestingly, no consistent differences emerged in objective measures of age-related health (for example, cognition, cortical thickness) in this group when compared to individuals within the ‘well-balanced’ profile (profile 3).
Previous studies have investigated the relevance of aggregated psychological factors for cognitive decline and dementia risk through a variable-centered approach, for example to identify the most robust subscales or traits that contribute to latent variables defining a given psychological construct, and have shown associations with cognitive status or brain pathology13,31. Our approach differs in that we used a person-centered approach. This approach could be beneficial to help identify groups of individuals who may be at greater risk of age-related decline. First, our findings reveal that having a ‘well-balanced’ psychological profile (profile 3), with moderately high protective and moderately low risk factors is related to better cognitive and mental health across all measured indicators. These associations were observed in middle-aged and older adults, which reinforces the relevance of considering the equilibrium of a broad range of psychological aspects as determinants of mental, cognitive and brain health in adulthood and advanced age.
Second, although it is increasingly acknowledged that high distress and depressive symptoms, as well as cognitive or sleep complaints (which are more characteristic of our profile 2), may indicate higher risk for future decline or dementia, evaluations in the clinical context rarely include assessments of protective factors (for example, high purpose in life). However, although individuals that resemble our profile 1 participants may not present with high anxiety/stress-related symptoms or cognitive complaints, they appear to be the closest to having ‘classical dementia risk’ (that is, lower cognitive performance, lower engagement in beneficial lifestyles and greater cortical atrophy). Combined, these findings highlight the importance of conducting comprehensive psychological evaluations, including both assessment of ‘risk’ as well as ‘protective’ factors, when aiming to estimate an individual’s risk profile. The above observations may also have implications for future interventions designed to prevent cognitive impairment and dementia. First, they provide new pathways for more personalized interventions based on the psychological profile of individuals. For example, individuals with profiles compatible with our profile 1 could benefit most from psychological therapies that include (re)-identification of valued behavior and life purposes such as acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT)32, whereas others (for example, meeting criteria for profile 2) may have a better response to therapies directed at reducing distress-related symptoms, which have also recently shown to entail potential benefit33,34.
Finally, our findings highlight that psychological profiles are differentially associated with engagement in other modifiable factors and lifestyles previously related to risk or protection for dementia. While accumulating evidence suggests that multidomain interventions that target modifiable factors and lifestyles show greatest promise for prevention35,36, lack of adherence and personalization are still important challenges faced by these approaches. Our findings could enhance preventative intervention initiatives by (1) offering psychological components as potentially modifiable intervention targets, and (2) guiding and deepening personalized interventions tailored for specific groups. From a mechanistic perspective, exploring whether modifying psychological profiles can drive lifestyle change, hence acting as potential intervention enhancers, would be a promising line of investigation.
Our study has several limitations that should be considered in further research in the field. First although we included a middle-aged and an older adult sample and the overall results were independently replicated, we only explored longitudinal associations with cognition and brain changes in the former. Second, we did not apply multiple comparisons corrections to our planned pairwise comparisons, as our aim was to uncover potential patterns and relationships rather than validate definitive hypotheses. Although this approach may increase the risk of type I errors, it enables the identification of relationships that warrant further investigation. Future studies with larger sample sizes are needed to further validate these initial findings. Third, despite reporting associations between psychological profiles and cortical atrophy, we did not include specific dementia (in particular, Alzheimer’s disease) biomarkers. Previous studies focusing on specific psychological characteristics have found associations in this regard5,8, so future prospective investigations should be undertaken to investigate whether psychological profiles also reflect these associations. We used domains that were available in both cohorts to generate the psychological profiles, so additional domains that have been reported as psychological risk (for example, pessimism37) or protective factors (for example, mindfulness13) were not captured. Finally, although our person-centered approach has some advantages as described above, it also reflected some apparent inconsistencies regarding the specific psychological feature of self-reflection. Individuals with profile 2 exhibited higher scores in self-reflection, which refers to the active evaluation of thoughts, feelings and behaviors, and has recently been associated with better global cognition and more preserved brain metabolism after adjusting for brooding12. However, self-reflection is also associated with high levels of brooding38, which may explain why in our study it aggregates in this ‘high negative’ profile and is lower in the ‘well-balanced’ one. Future work could examine the unique contribution of ‘self-reflection’ in the profiles.
In summary, the present study focusing on psychological profiling has identified two divergent patterns of associations that may suggest two distinct paths for cognitive impairment or dementia risk. A low positive profile, characterized by low purpose in life and lower levels of some personality characteristics (conscientiousness, openness to experience, extraversion and agreeableness), was related to worse cognition (more clearly observable in older adults), higher brain atrophy (already observable in middle age) and lower engagement in protective lifestyles. A high negative profile, characterized by greater proneness to distress and negative thinking styles, may increase the risk of cognitive impairment/dementia through a psycho-affective pathway, which includes expressing symptoms of depression and anxiety, cognitive complaints, loneliness and poor sleep health. Our findings highlight the need to consider both risk profile patterns when designing future, more personalized preventive strategies.
Data were utilized from participants enrolled in two European cohorts: BBHI (https://bbhi.cat/en) and Medit-Ageing (Silver Santé Study (public name): https://silversantestudy.eu/).
BBHI is an ongoing longitudinal cohort study that launched in 2017, with the primary aim of investigating the determinants of mental and brain health in healthy middle-aged and older adults. At study commencement, adults aged 40 to 65 years were eligible if they had no neurological or psychiatric disorders, unstable medical diagnoses, or cognitive impairment based on a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment39,40. For the current study, participants with data available on psychological characteristics that matched the ones administered in Medit-Ageing (see below) and who also completed cognitive, brain imaging, lifestyle and/or mental health assessments at baseline were included. BBHI was approved by the Unió Catalana d’Hospitals ethics committee (approval references CEIC 17/06 and CEI 18/07). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.
Medit-Ageing aims to explore the determinants of mental health and well-being in older adults. It comprises two randomized controlled trials, Age-Well and SCD-Well, with baseline data from these trials utilized in the present study41,42. All Age-Well participants were recruited from the general population, aged 65 years or older, native French speakers, retired for at least one year, and received at least seven years of education, with recruitment beginning in late 2016 and ending in May 201841. In SCD-Well, participants were recruited through memory clinics at four European centers (London, UK; Lyon, France; Cologne, Germany; Barcelona, Spain), met research criteria for subjective cognitive decline (that is, self-perceived decline in cognitive capacity but normal performance on standardized cognitive tests used to classify mild cognitive impairment or prodromal AD43), and were aged 60 or older, with recruitment occurring from March 2017 to January 201842. Participants in Age-Well and SCD-Well had no evidence of major neurological or psychiatric disorders and performed within normal ranges on standardized cognitive tests. Baseline data from both trials were combined to create a single cohort (that is, Medit-Ageing) for the present study. Both trials were approved by local ethics committees (Age-Well: CPP Nord-Ouest III, Caen; EudraCT: 2016-002441-36; IDRCB: 2016-A01767-44; SCD-Well: London, UK (Queen Square Research Ethics Committee: no. 17/LO/0056 and Health Research Authority IRAS project ID: 213008); Lyon, France (Comité de Protection des Personnes Sud-Est II Groupement Hospitalier Est: no. 2016-30-1 and Agence Nationale de Sécurité du Médicament et des Produits de Santé: IDRCB 2016-A01298-43); Cologne, Germany (Ethikkommission der Medizinischen Fakultät der Universität zu Köln: no. 17-059); and Barcelona, Spain (Comité Etico de Investigacion Clinica del Hospital Clinic de Barcelona: no. HCB/2017/0062)) and were registered on ClinicalTrials.gov (Age-Well, NCT02977819; SCD-Well, NCT03005652). All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation.
In both BBHI and Medit-Ageing, the participants completed self-report questionnaires to assess a range of psychological characteristics. For the current study, the selection of psychological characteristics followed two criteria: first, their relevance was established based on existing evidence indicating their potential impact on cognition, Alzheimer’s disease pathology and/or dementia; second, they needed to be available in both cohorts. Following these criteria, brooding, worry and neuroticism were selected as psychological characteristics associated with heightened risk, whilst purpose in life, conscientiousness, openness to experience, extraversion, agreeableness and self-reflection were identified as protective psychological characteristics. Supplementary Table 7 provides details on the questionnaires used to measure these characteristics in both cohorts.
Depression and anxiety symptoms were assessed in both cohorts. In BBHI, the ‘depression’ and ‘anxiety’ subscales of the Depression, Anxiety and Stress scale (DASS; possible range 0 to 21) were used44. In Medit-Ageing, depressive symptoms were assessed using the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; possible range 0 to 15)45, and anxiety symptoms examined via the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) trait sub-scale (possible range 20 to 80)46. Anxiety and depression are regarded as distal outcomes as they represent symptoms of clinical conditions, whereas the selected psychological characteristics act as proposed antecedents. For example, although worry is considered the cognitive component of anxiety, empirical evidence supports its distinction as an independent construct. Research suggests a unidirectional relationship between worry and anxiety, with a strong positive effect of worry on anxiety but no effect in the opposite direction47.
The PACC5 is a validated global cognitive composite sensitive to detecting and tracking preclinical Alzheimer’s disease-related decline48. It comprises two episodic memory measures and one measure of executive function, semantic memory and global cognition, and allows the flexibility to select specific tests within each domain48. In Medit-Ageing and BBHI, the four-item PACC5abridged was created49, as only one episodic memory measure was available in SCD-Well. PACC5abridged scores were computed separately in each cohort by averaging z-transformed cognitive test scores, with scores only calculable when data were available for all constituent measures. Higher PACC5abridged scores indicate better cognition. Supplementary Table 8 and the Supplementary Methods detail the specific neuropsychological tests included in PACC5abridged and the procedure used to calculate the composite in each cohort.
In addition to objective cognition, subjective perception of cognitive health was examined in BBHI and Medit-Ageing using the Neuro-QoL (possible range 12 to 60)50 and McNair Cognitive Difficulties Scale (CDS; possible range 0 to 156)51, respectively. Higher Neuro-QoL and lower McNair CDS scores indicate greater perceived cognitive difficulties.
The late-life LIBRA is a poly-environmental risk score for cognitive functioning and dementia risk52. It typically comprises ten health and lifestyle factors (depression, coronary heart disease, diabetes, hypercholesterolemia, smoking, physical inactivity, renal disease, low-to-moderate alcohol use, high cognitive activity and healthy diet), which receive weights based on their relative risk52. As depressive symptoms are an outcome in the current study and are included as a covariate in sensitivity analyses, a nine-item LIBRA index was derived by removing the depression-weighted score12. To compute the nine-item LIBRA, the weights of the remaining nine factors were summed to yield LIBRA scores (possible range −5.9 to 7.4). Higher LIBRA scores indicate poorer health and lifestyle behaviors. The LIBRA was calculable only in BBHI, because 44.4% of the LIBRA components were assessed differently in the two studies (Age-Well and SCD-Well) comprising Medit-Ageing. This made it infeasible to compute a LIBRA score in Medit-Ageing that was comparable across participants. Supplementary Table 9 contains further details on the risk and protective factors included in the LIBRA in BBHI.
In addition to the health and lifestyle factors included in the LIBRA score, loneliness (measured with the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale; possible range 3 to 9)53 and perception of sleep quality (measured in BBHI using the Jenkins scale (possible range 4 to 24)54 and in Medit-Ageing using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (possible range 0 to 21)55) were assessed in both cohorts. In BBHI, social interaction engagement was also examined via the LUBBEN social network scale (LSNS; possible range 0 to 60)56.
For the BBHI cohort, MRI images were acquired in a 3T Siemens scanner (MAGNETOM Prisma) with a 32-channel head coil at the Unitat d’Imatge per Ressonància Magnètica IDIBAPS (Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi I Sunyer) at the Hospital Clínic de Barcelona. For all participants, a high-resolution T1-weighted (T1w) structural image was obtained with a magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) three-dimensional protocol (repetition time (TR) = 2,400 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.22 ms, inversion time = 1,000 ms, field of view (FOV) = 256 mm, 0.8-mm isotropic voxel). Additionally, a high-resolution three-dimensional SPACE T2-weighted (T2w) acquisition was taken (TR = 3,200 ms, TE = 563 ms, flip angle = 120°, 0.8-mm isotropic voxel, FOV = 256 mm). All acquisitions were examined by a senior neuroradiologist to detect any clinically significant pathology. All MRI data were then visually inspected before analysis to ensure that they did not contain artifacts or excessive motion.
Individual T1w images for each time point were automatically processed with FreeSurfer version 6.0 (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) to obtain maps of cortical thickness, following a previously described procedure57. The processing stream was run with default parameters, except for the addition of the T2w images for the improvement of pial surfaces reconstruction. All images obtained a high Euler score (that is, 2), indicating that the reconstructions consisted of smooth surfaces, with no holes or handles detected in the pial or the white matter surfaces, at either time point. Next, within-subject template volumes and longitudinal files were created for each participant and time point through the longitudinal stream58. At each step, the results were visually inspected following gross quality-control measures, and no manual editing was performed. The symmetrized percentage change was used as a robust longitudinal measure of cortical thickness, computed as the percentage of change corrected for the average cortical thickness values at each time point. Before statistical analysis, cortical thickness maps were smoothed using a two-dimensional Gaussian kernel of 15-mm full-width at half-maximum. We carried out a vertex-wise one-factor/three-level general linear model provided by FreeSurfer to study profile differences regarding cortical thickness loss (that is, symmetrized percent change). The primary model was adjusted for baseline age, sex and education, with a subsequent model including additional adjustments for symptoms of anxiety and depression. Sensitivity analyses included cognitive change as an additional covariate. Additionally, we obtained longitudinal maps of atrophy for the whole sample. Multiple comparisons correction of whole-brain vertices was performed by computing P values for contiguous clusters of vertices based on Monte-Carlo Null-Z simulations and permutation (with 10,000 iterations per simulation). This method assigns a P value to each resulting cluster. Consequently, we used a cluster-forming threshold of P < 0.05 and a cluster significance threshold of P < 0.05 in all models.
In the initial analysis stage, LPA was conducted on the nine psychological characteristics, using data from nine questionnaires at baseline from all participants. Analyses were conducted separately for each cohort (BBHI and Medit-Ageing), enabling a comparative assessment and validation of the profile structures.
Various model fit statistics were considered to determine the optimal profile solution. The VLMR-LRT and the bootstrap likelihood ratio test (B-LRT) were performed to identify the best fitting model. Because no prior hypotheses were made regarding the number of profiles that would arise, analyses were conducted starting with a two-profile model and increasing the number of profiles by one until the VLMR-LRT became non-significant. To confirm the K-1 model results, a parametric bootstrap procedure was employed using the B-LRT. This was further supplemented by the evaluation of common fit indices, including the Akaike information criterion (AIC), the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), sample size-adjusted BIC, and entropy values. The VLMR-LRT and B-LRT tests compare K (current model with K number of profiles) and K – 1 (model with one less profile) models, where a significant P value indicates that the K model provides a better fit compared to the model with one less profile. Conversely, P ≥ 0.05 indicates that the model with one less profile is preferred, as it provides a better fit for the data and is more parsimonious. Lower AIC and BIC values indicate a better fitting model, while higher entropy values suggest higher classification accuracy. In the scenario of multiple possible profile solutions, model interpretability and clinical/theoretical relevance were considered. Furthermore, as a profile-utility criterion, profiles needed to include at least 5% of the total sample. After determining the optimal profile solution within each cohort, participants were assigned to the profile for which they exhibited the highest probability of membership, for use in subsequent analyses.
Differences in demographic characteristics (that is, age, sex, education, ethnicity and APOE ε4 genotype) were examined between cohorts and across profiles within cohorts. Of note, a direct comparison of educational attainment between cohorts was not possible due to differences in the assessment methods for education in BBHI and Medit-Ageing.
After identifying the optimal profile solution within each cohort, regression models were constructed to explore the cross-sectional associations between psychological profiles and mental, cognitive and brain health, along with other lifestyle-related behaviors linked to dementia risk. For the latter, the components included in the LIBRA composite were analyzed separately as continuous variables when available (instead of the dichotomized version that were used to calculate the composite). These analyses were conducted separately for each outcome and in each cohort independently. All analyses were adjusted for age, sex and education, and in Medit-Ageing, study (that is, Age-Well/SCD-Well) was also included as a covariate. In sensitivity analyses, anxiety and depressive symptoms were included as additional covariates. Associations between psychological profile membership and all outcomes were first evaluated using the anova command in R to determine whether psychological profile membership was related to the selected outcome. Following identification of an association, adjusted planned pairwise comparisons were performed using the emmeans function.
To assess cognitive changes across two time points, linear mixed-effects models were fitted, with PACC5abridged as the dependent variable. These models included random intercepts at the subject level and time coded as a dummy variable (that is, visit). Initially, a model was run to investigate general temporal change, representing the overall change in cognition over time regardless of profile group. Subsequently, a separate model was run, introducing a time-by-profile interaction to examine differences in cognitive change (that is, slopes) over time among the three latent profiles. All analyses were adjusted for age at baseline, sex, education and the number of months between the two visits. These analyses were restricted to a BBHI subsample comprising participants who underwent two neuropsychological evaluations.
All baseline cross-sectional analyses were conducted again using data from the BBHI subsample with longitudinal data to assess the comparability of findings with the total BBHI sample. Additionally, cross-sectional analyses (that is, regression models) using visit two data were performed, as described above, to explore whether the associations observed between psychological profiles and cognition at baseline were maintained at follow-up.
The LPA was performed in Mplus (version 8.3). R (version 4.3.1) was used to conduct all other analyses, including regressions using the lm function, and planned pairwise comparisons using the emmeans package. All statistical tests were two-sided.
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this Article.
For BBHI, the data are not publicly accessible. However, the authors welcome inquiries from interested researchers and will accommodate all reasonable and scientifically justified requests for data access, providing the raw data when needed. Queries should be addressed to D. Bartrés-Faz. For Age-Well and SCD-Well, the material can be mobilized on request following a formal data-sharing agreement and approval by the consortium and executive committee, under the conditions and modalities defined in the Medit-Ageing Charter by any research team belonging to an Academic, for carrying out a scientific research project relating to the scientific theme of mental health and well-being in older people. The material may also be mobilized by non-academic third parties, under conditions, in particular financial, which will be established by separate agreement between lnserm and the said third party. Information and access to the data request form can be found at https://silversantestudy.eu/2020/09/25/data-sharing.
Mental Health of Older Adults (World Health Organization, 2023); https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-health-of-older-adults
Prince, M. et al. World Alzheimer Report 2015. The Global Impact of Dementia: An Analysis of Prevalence, Incidence, Cost and Trends (Alzheimer’s Disease International, 2015).
Livingston, G. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2024 report of the Lancet standing Commission. Lancet 404, 572–628 (2024).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Bartrés-Faz, D., Solé-Padullés, C. & Marchant, N. L. Cognitive aging and dementia prevention: the time for psychology? Aging (Albany NY) 15, 889–891 (2023).
PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Marchant, N. L. et al. Repetitive negative thinking is associated with amyloid, tau and cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement. 16, 1054–1064 (2020).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Schlosser, M., Demnitz-King, H., Whitfield, T., Wirth, M. & Marchant, N. L. Repetitive negative thinking is associated with subjective cognitive decline in older adults: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry 20, 500 (2020).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Karim, H. T. et al. Aging faster: worry and rumination in late life are associated with greater brain age. Neurobiol. Aging 101, 13–21 (2021).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Terracciano, A. et al. Personality associations with amyloid and tau: results from the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging and Meta-Analysis. Biol. Psychiatry 91, 359–369 (2022).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Nabe-Nielsen, K. et al. Perceived stress and dementia: results from the Copenhagen city heart study. Aging Ment. Health 24, 1828–1836 (2020).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Bell, G., Singham, T., Saunders, R., John, A. & Stott, J. Positive psychological constructs and association with reduced risk of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res. Rev. 77, 101594 (2022).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Bartrés-Faz, D., Cattaneo, G., Solana, J., Tormos, J. M. & Pascual-Leone, A. Meaning in life: resilience beyond reserve. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 10, 47 (2018).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Demnitz-King, H. et al. Association of self-reflection with cognition and brain health in cognitively unimpaired older adults. Neurology 99, e1422–e1431 (2022).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Strikwerda-Brown, C. et al. Trait mindfulness is associated with less amyloid, tau, and cognitive decline in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Biol. Psychiatry Glob. Open Sci. 3, 130–138 (2022).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Kusurkar, R. A. et al. ‘One size does not fit all’: the value of person-centred analysis in health professions education research. Perspect. Med. Educ. 10, 245–251 (2021).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Bergman, L. R. & Magnusson, D. A person-oriented approach in research on developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 291–319 (1997).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Sutin, Dr. A. R. et al. Sense of meaning and purpose in life and risk of incident dementia: new data and meta-analysis. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 105, 104847 (2023).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Aschwanden, D. et al. Is personality associated with dementia risk? A meta-analytic investigation. Ageing Res. Rev. 67, 101269 (2021).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Boyle, P. A. et al. Effect of purpose in life on the relation between Alzheimer disease pathologic changes on cognitive function in advanced age. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 499–504 (2012).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Abellaneda-Pérez, K. et al. Purpose in life promotes resilience to age-related brain burden in middle-aged adults. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 15, 49 (2023).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Boreham, I. D. & Schutte, N. S. The relationship between purpose in life and depression and anxiety: a meta-analysis. J. Clin. Psychol. 79, 2736–2767 (2023).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Willroth, E. C. et al. Well-being and cognitive resilience to dementia-related neuropathology. Psychol. Sci. 34, 283–297 (2023).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Dickerson, B. C. et al. The cortical signature of Alzheimer’s disease: regionally specific cortical thinning relates to symptom severity in very mild to mild AD dementia and is detectable in asymptomatic amyloid-positive individuals. Cereb. Cortex 19, 497–510 (2009).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Storsve, A. B. et al. Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. J. Neurosci. 34, 8488–8498 (2014).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Marchant, N. L. & Howard, R. J. Cognitive debt and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 44, 755–770 (2015).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Terracciano, A. et al. Is neuroticism differentially associated with risk of Alzheimer’s disease, vascular dementia and frontotemporal dementia? J. Psychiatric Res. 138, 34–40 (2021).
Article  Google Scholar 
Pike, K. E., Cavuoto, M. G., Li, L., Wright, B. J. & Kinsella, G. J. Subjective cognitive decline: level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol. Rev. 32, 703–735 (2022).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Yang, L. et al. Depression, depression treatments, and risk of incident dementia: a prospective cohort study of 354,313 participants. Biol. Psychiatry 93, 802–809 (2023).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Santabárbara, J. et al. Does anxiety increase the risk of all-cause dementia? An updated meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. J. Clin. Med. 9, 1791 (2020).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Shi, L. et al. Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 40, 4–16 (2018).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Qiao, L. et al. Association between loneliness and dementia risk: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16, 899814 (2022).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Pichet Binette, A. et al. Amyloid and tau pathology associations with personality traits, neuropsychiatric symptoms, and cognitive lifestyle in the preclinical phases of sporadic and autosomal dominant Alzheimer’s disease. Biol. Psychiatry 89, 776–785 (2021).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Stenhoff, A., Steadman, L., Nevitt, S., Benson, L. & White, R. G. Acceptance and commitment therapy and subjective wellbeing: a systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials in adults. J. Contextual Behav. Sci. 18, 256–272 (2020).
Article  Google Scholar 
John, A. et al. Associations between psychological therapy outcomes for depression and incidence of dementia. Psychol. Med. 53, 4869–4879 (2023).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Stott, J. et al. Associations between psychological intervention for anxiety disorders and risk of dementia: a prospective cohort study using national health-care records data in England. Lancet Healthy Longev. 4, e12–e22 (2023).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Ngandu, T. et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet 385, 2255–2263 (2015).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Kivipelto, M. et al. World-Wide FINGERS Network: a global approach to risk reduction and prevention of dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 16, 1078–1094 (2020).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Sutin, A. R., Stephan, Y. & Terracciano, A. Psychological distress, self-beliefs, and risk of cognitive impairment and dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 65, 1041–1050 (2018).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R. & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Rumination reconsidered: a psychometric analysis. Cogn. Ther. Res. 27, 247–259 (2003).
Article  Google Scholar 
Cattaneo, G. et al. The Barcelona Brain Health Initiative: a cohort study to define and promote determinants of brain health. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10, 321 (2018).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Cattaneo, G. et al. ‘Guttmann Cognitest’®, preliminary validation of a digital solution to test cognitive performance. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14, 987891 (2022).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Poisnel, G. et al. The Age-Well randomized controlled trial of the Medit-Ageing European project: effect of meditation or foreign language training on brain and mental health in older adults. Alzheimers Dement. (N. Y.) 4, 714–723 (2018).
Google Scholar 
Marchant, N. L. et al. The SCD-Well randomized controlled trial: effects of a mindfulness-based intervention versus health education on mental health in patients with subjective cognitive decline (SCD). Alzheimers Dement (N. Y.) 4, 737–745 (2018).
Google Scholar 
Jessen, F. et al. A conceptual framework for research on subjective cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 10, 844–852 (2014).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Brown, T. A., Chorpita, B. F., Korotitsch, W. & Barlow, D. H. Psychometric properties of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) in clinical samples. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 79–89 (1997).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Sheikh, J. I. & Yesavage, J. A. Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS): recent evidence and development of a shorter version. Clin. Gerontol. J. Aging Ment. Health 5, 165–173 (1986).
Google Scholar 
Spielberger, C. D., Gonzalez-Reigosa, F., Martinez-Urrutia, A., Natalicio, L. F. & Natalicio, D. S. The state-trait anxiety inventory. Revista Interamericana de Psicologia/Interam. J. Psychol. 5, 145–158 (1971).
Google Scholar 
Gana, K., Martin, B. & Canouet, M. D. Worry and anxiety: is there a causal relationship? Psychopathology 34, 221–229 (2001).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Papp, K. V., Rentz, D. M., Orlovsky, I., Sperling, R. A. & Mormino, E. C. Optimizing the preclinical Alzheimer’s cognitive composite with semantic processing: the PACC5. Alzheimers Dement. (N. Y.) 3, 668–677 (2017).
Google Scholar 
Whitfield, T. et al. Effects of a mindfulness-based versus a health self-management intervention on objective cognitive performance in older adults with subjective cognitive decline (SCD): a secondary analysis of the SCD-Well randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 14, 125 (2022).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Cella, D. et al. Neuro-QOL: brief measures of health-related quality of life for clinical research in neurology. Neurology 78, 1860–1867 (2012).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
McNair, D. & Kahn, R. Self-assessment of cognitive deficits. Assess. Geriatr. Psychopharmacol. 137, 143 (1983).
Google Scholar 
Deckers, K. et al. Long‐term dementia risk prediction by the LIBRA score: a 30‐year follow‐up of the CAIDE study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 35, 195–203 (2020).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C. & Cacioppo, J. T. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys. Res. Aging 26, 655–672 (2004).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Jenkins, C. D., Stanton, B.-A., Niemcryk, S. J. & Rose, R. M. A scale for the estimation of sleep problems in clinical research. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 41, 313–321 (1988).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R. & Kupfer, D. J. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213 (1989).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Lubben, J. E. Assessing social networks among elderly populations. Fam. Community Health 11, 42–52 (1988).
Article  Google Scholar 
Fischl, B. & Dale, A. M. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 97, 11050–11055 (2000).
Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Reuter, M. & Fischl, B. Avoiding asymmetry-induced bias in longitudinal image processing. Neuroimage 57, 19–21 (2011).
Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Download references
The research leading to these results has received funding from ‘la Caixa’ Foundation (grant no. LCF/PR/PR16/11110004), the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation program (grant no. 667696) and from Institut Guttmann and Fundació Abertis. D.B.-F. was funded by grant PID2022-137234OB-100 funded by MICIU/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and ‘ERDF/EU’, a ‘Programa de Estancias de Movilidad en Centros Extrangeros’, a Research Stay Grant (PRX21/00690) funded by the Spanish Ministry of Universities, and an ICREA Academia 2019 research grant from the Catalan Government. C.G. and J.S.-S. are partially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PID2022-139298OA-C22). L.V.-A. was partially funded by a Margarita Salas Grant from the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities of the Government of Spain (Next Generation EU program) and Instituto de Salud Carlos III (Sara Borrell Grant CD23/00235). J.G. was supported by a Young Researcher Grant 2019–2022 from the Fondation Alzheimer and Fondation de France. O.K. received funding from the Secrétariat d’État à la Formation, à la Recherche et à l’Innovation (SEFRI) under contract no. 15.0336 in the context of the European project ‘Medit-Ageing’.
A list of members and their affiliations appears in the Supplementary Information.
These authors contributed equally: David Bartrés-Faz, Harriet Demnitz-King.
Department of Medicine, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences and Institute of Neurosciences, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
David Bartrés-Faz, María Cabello-Toscano & Lídia Vaqué-Alcázar
Institut de Recerca Biomèdica August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain
David Bartrés-Faz, María Cabello-Toscano & Lídia Vaqué-Alcázar
Institut Guttmann, Institut Universitari de Neurorehabilitació adscrit a la Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
David Bartrés-Faz, María Cabello-Toscano, Gabriele Cattaneo, Javier Sánchez-Solana & José M. Tormos
Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, UK
Harriet Demnitz-King, Natalie L. Marchant, Marco Schlosser, Zuzana Walker & Tim Whitfield
Sant Pau Memory Unit, Department of Neurology, Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques Sant Pau-Hospital de Sant Pau, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Lídia Vaqué-Alcázar
CORE Data Lab, Centre for Outcomes and Research Effectiveness, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Rob Saunders
UNICAEN, INSERM, U1237, PhIND ‘Physiopathology and Imaging of Neurological Disorders’, NeuroPresage Team, GIP Cyceron, Normandie Université, Caen, France
Edelweiss Touron, Julie Gonneaud, Gäel Chételat, Claire André, Anne Chocat, Sophie Dautricourt, Marion Delarue, Eglantine Ferrand Devouge, Elizabeth Kuhn, Brigitte Landeau, Valérie Lefranc, Florence Mezenge, Valentin Ourry, Géraldine Poisnel, Anne Quillard & Géraldine Rauchs
Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Bellaterra, Spain
Gabriele Cattaneo & Javier Sánchez-Solana
Fundació Institut d’Investigació en Ciències de la Salut Germans Trias i Pujol, Barcelona, Spain
Gabriele Cattaneo & Javier Sánchez-Solana
Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Olga Klimecki
Biological Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, Technische Universität Dresden, Dresden, Germany
Olga Klimecki
Clinical Psychology, Geneva, Switzerland
Olga Klimecki
Diagnostic per la Imatge Hospital Clinic Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Núria Bargalló
Universitat de Barcelona and CIBERSAM, Barcelona, Spain
Núria Bargalló
Universidad Católica de Valencia, Valencia, Spain
José M. Tormos
Hinda and Arthur Marcus Institute for Aging Research and Deanna and Sidney Wolk Center for Memory Health, Hebrew SeniorLife, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Álvaro Pascual-Leone
Department of Neurology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
Álvaro Pascual-Leone
Hospices Civils de Lyon, Institut du Vieillissement, CRC Vieillissement-Cerveau-Fragilite, Lyon, France
Romain Bachelet & Karine Goldet
Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland; Laboratory for Behavioral Neurology and Imaging of Cognition, Department of Neuroscience, Medical School, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Sebastian Baez Lugo & Patrik Vuilleumier
GIGA-CRC in Vivo Imaging, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
Fabienne Collette
Psychology and Neuroscience of Cognition, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
Fabienne Collette
Fund for Scientific Research FNRS, Brussels, Belgium
Fabienne Collette
Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Hospital Clinic, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Spain
Nina Coll-Padros & José Luis Molinuevo
Normandie Univ, UNICAEN, PSL Université, EPHE, INSERM, U1077, CHU de Caen, GIP Cyceron, NIMH, Caen, France
Vincent De La Sayette & Valentin Ourry
Service de Neurologie, CHU de Caen, Caen, France
Vincent De La Sayette
Department of General Practice, Normandie Université, UNIROUEN, Rouen, France
Eglantine Ferrand Devouge
Rouen University Hospital, Centre d’Investigation Clinique-Centre de Ressources Biologiques (CIC-CRB) 1404, Rouen, France
Eglantine Ferrand Devouge
EUCLID/F-CRIN Clinical Trials Platform, Bordeaux Population Health Center, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
Eric Frison
Service d’Information Médicale, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
Eric Frison
Department of Psychiatry, Medical Faculty, University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
Frank Jessen, Dix Meiberth & Ann-Katrin Schild
German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Bonn, Germany
Frank Jessen
Excellence Cluster on Cellular Stress Responses in Aging-Associated Diseases (CECAD), University of Cologne, Cologne, Germany
Frank Jessen
Hospices Civils de Lyon, Institut du Vieillissement, CRC Vieillissement-Cerveau-Fragilité, Lyon, France
Pierre Krolak-Salmon
Lyon Neuroscience Research Center Inserm U1028, Lyon University, Lyon, France
Antoine Lutz
Haute Autorité de Santé, Saint-Denis, France
Laurence Michel
Barcelonaβeta Brain Research Center (BBRC), Pasqual Maragall Foundation, Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Fragilidad y Envejecimiento Saludable (CIBER FES), Madrid, Spain
José Luis Molinuevo
Inserm UMR-S U1237, GIP Cyceron, Université de Caen-Normandie, Caen, France
Géraldine Poisnel & Géraldine Rauchs
Division of Nuclear Medicine, Department of Medical Physics, Cyclotron Research Center, University Hospital of Liège, University of Liège, Liège, Belgium
Eric Salmon
Carré International, Bonn, Germany
Corinne Schimmer
Inserm Transfert, Paris, France
Delphine Smagghe
Minerva Health & Care Communications Ltd, Andover, UK
Rhonda Smith
Geneva School of Social Sciences, and Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
Marco Schlosser
Institut de Santé Publique, d’Epidémiologie et de Développement, Université de Bordeaux, Bordeaux, France
Cédrick Wallet
CIC-EC1401/EUCLID, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale, Bordeaux, France
Cédrick Wallet
Essex Partnership University NHS Foundation Trust, Essex, UK
Zuzana Walker
German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Dresden, Germany
Miranka Wirth
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
N.L.M., H.D.-K. and D.B.-F. conceptualized the work and developed the methodology. H.D.-K., M.C.-T., R.S. and L.V.-A. carried out formal statistical analyses. N.L.M., D.B.-F., G. Chételat and A.P.-L. supervised the work. E.T., G. Cattaneo, J.G., O.K., N.B., J.S.-S. and J.M.T. contributed to data collection, writing or reviewing, and approving the submitted version of the manuscript.
Correspondence to David Bartrés-Faz or Natalie L. Marchant.
O.K. has received honoraria for research, training and consulting related to meditation. The remaining authors declare no competing interests.
Nature Mental Health thanks Luca Cuffaro, Helmet T. Karim and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Tables 1–9, figure and methods.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Reprints and permissions
Bartrés-Faz, D., Demnitz-King, H., Cabello-Toscano, M. et al. Psychological profiles associated with mental, cognitive and brain health in middle-aged and older adults. Nat. Mental Health (2025). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44220-024-00361-8
Download citation
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s44220-024-00361-8
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
Advertisement
Nature Mental Health (Nat. Mental Health) ISSN 2731-6076 (online)
© 2025 Springer Nature Limited
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

source

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *